Make a follow-up of a student’s weekly discussion and respond with your opinion regarding to her post
Kayla Stephens
After World War 1, many countries, including the major countries, were struck with debt from the war. Many countries were broke. The Treaty of Versailles forced the opposing countries to pay for their wrong-doings, leaving these countries no money to care for the citizens. After the first world war, many countries felt it was best to keep to themselves and not intervene with foreign countries. As a result, each country relied on only themselves to get things back to normal. As a result, people turned to the promises of leaders in the countries leading to the rise of Mussolini in Italy and Hitler in Germany. Although one was socialist and one was fascist, both leaders gave promises of filling the stomachs of their citizens, and regaining their countries authority back. These governments worked hard to grow their militaries to ensure that they would never be destroyed again. Other countries saw the increase in military and knew it was against the rules of the treaty of Versailles, but they were reluctant to intervene with the threat of a second great war(Openstax, 2019). It is easy to say that if the allied countries would have acted sooner, when they saw the growth of military in Germany, Italy, and Japan the war could have been stopped before it had even began. If the Treaty of Versailles was taken seriously, the central powers could have disarmed the enemy before they got to their full strength.
Just line in World War I, Americans wanted to stay neutral in the situations happening in Europe. Also just like in World War 1, America aided the Axis powers by sending military supplies and other assistance. The Lend-Lease act was put into place, were the United States would lend military supplies to Europe in exchange for European land(Milestones: 1937-1945 – Office of the Historian, n.d.). The opposing powers saw this as a betrayal, just as they did in WWI.
References
Milestones: 1937-1945 – Office of the Historian. (n.d.). Retrieved October 2, 2022, from
https://history.state.gov/milestones/1937-1945/lend-lease#:%7E:text=During%20World%20War%20II%2C%20the,the%20war%20until%20December%201941
Links to an external site.
.
OpenStax. (2019).
U.S. history. OpenStax CNX. Retrieved from
https://cnx.org/contents/[emailprotected]:[emailprotected]/Introduction
SHOW MORE…
“Thinking Critically about Critical Thinking,” Brent D. Slife, Stephen Yanchar, Jeffrey Reber
After reading “Thinking Critically about Critical Thinking,” explain the benefits of using new critical thinking as opposed to older views of critical thinking using references from the article.
*Posts should contain a minimum of 125 words.
* References should be made using APA format.
Objectives Unacceptable Below Average Acceptable Above Average Exemplary Score
0 Points 20 Points
Student did not make any
post in the discussion board
Student posts were on time
0 Points 5 Points 10 Points 15 Points 20 Points
No reference to any course
reading
Makes reference to
assigned readings; attempts
to cite the source
Makes references to course
and/or outside reading
material but citations do not
conform to an acceptable
citation format
Refers to and properly cites
in APA format course and/or
outside reading in initial
posting only
Refers to and properly cites
in APA format either course
materials or external
readings in initial posts and
responses
0 Points 5 Points 10 Points 15 Points 20 Points
No postings for which to
evaluate language and
grammar
Poorly written initial posting
and responses including
frequent spelling, structure,
and/or grammar errors
Communicates in friendly,
courteous, and helpful
manner with some spelling,
grammatical, and/or
structural errors
Contributes valuable
information with minor
grammatical or structural
errors
No spelling, structure, or
grammar errors in any
posting; Contributes to
discussion with clear,
concise comments
0 Points 5 Points 10 Points 15 Points 20 Points
No initial posting
Response was not on topic,
the message was unrelated
to assignment, and post was
less than 150 words
The initial posting did not
adequately address the
question posed in the forum;
superficial thought and
preparation
Initial posting demonstrates
sincere reflection and
answers most aspects of the
forum; full development
Initial posting reveals a solid
understanding of all aspects
of the task; uses factually
and relevand information;
and the length of the posting
is at least 150 words
0 Points 5 Points 10 Points 15 Points 20 Points
Student did not participate in
this forum
Student participated on but
did not respond to other
student posts
Student participated but only
responded to one
Student participated and
commented on two other
student’s posts
Student actively
participated, responded to at
least two other students’
posts, and replied to other
students’ comments on their
original post.
Total Score 0
Frequency of
Participation
Reference to
Course Readings
Language and
Grammar
DISCUSSION FORUM RUBRIC
Timeliness of
Forum Posting
Quality of Initial
Posting Introduction
1
Thinking Critically about Critical Thinking:
An Introduction
Brent D. Slife, Stephen Yanchar, Jeffrey Reber
Critical thinking has long been acclaimed as an essential skill for any academic or
professional endeavor. Within psychology, especially, critical thinking has been consistently
championed for all students and professionals (Benjafield, 1994; Bensley, 1998; Griggs, Jackson,
Marek, & Christopher, 1998; Halpern, 1998; Halpern & Nummedal, 1995; Levy, 1997; Meltzoff,
1998; Smith, 2002; Yanchar & Slife, in press). Psychologists are taught early in their careers to
use their research findings to critically examine common myths and urban legends as well as
debunk false beliefs and advertising ploys (e.g., Tavris, 2001). Yet, in spite of this obvious
emphasis, psychologists do not typically subject psychology itself to critical evaluation. As
many outside observers of psychology have noted (e.g., Bohman, 1993; MacIntyre, 1984;
Taylor, 1985), mainstream psychologists often take for granted their philosophies, research
methods, and professional practices. Even the tacit assumptions that guide psychological
research on critical thinking are rarely critically analyzed or systematically examined. Why?
A primary reason for this neglect is that many psychologists have misunderstood critical
thinking. Critical thinking has too often been mistaken for rigorous thinking. Rigorous thinking
is frequently identified with scientific analytic reasoning (Dick, 1991, p. 84), which focuses on
methodological concerns such as quality of research design, appropriateness of statistical
analyses, and rigor of general reasoning. Psychologists are well known to engage skillfully in
this type of thinking. Rigorous reasoning and methods are used not only to conduct
psychological investigations but also to administer therapeutic practice. With few exceptions,
investigators are supposed to follow the logic of their science, and clients are supposed to follow
Introduction
2
the rationality of their therapists. This commitment to rigorous reasoning is so widespread that
psychologists conceptualize most of their activities in these terms. Students of psychology are
taught this type of rigor in virtually all their courses.
One of the main problems with this sort of rigorous thinking is that it selectively excludes
certain topics from critical examination. For example, scientific reasoning and methods are often
themselves taken for granted, exempting from critical analysis one of the core activities of
psychologists. Philosophers of science point to many hidden assumptions and values in
scientific methods and practices (Bernstein, 1983; Bem & de Jong, 1997; Bohman, 1993; Curd &
Cover, 1998; Slife & Williams, 1995; Taylor, 1985; Toulmin, 1972). Yet, these assumptions and
values are rarely included in texts or discussions of research and therapy methods in psychology.
Consequently, the foundations of these methods and these practices are not themselves subjected
to critical scrutiny.
This volume attempts to rectify such oversight and selectivity. It does so by adopting a
conception of critical thinking that a number of philosophers and educators have contended is
broader and deeper than previous conceptions. Perhaps most notably, recognized critical
thinking theorists and researchers, such as Stephen Brookfield (1987) from education and
Richard Paul (Paul & Elder, 2001) and Robert Ennis (1982) from philosophy, have emphasized a
reformulation of critical thinking that moves beyond mere scientific analytic reasoning. If
applied to psychology, this approach would inevitably lead to a critical analysis of all aspects of
the discipline, including psychological research on critical thinking itself.
This reformulation of critical thinking has two parts. The first requires knowledge of the
assumptions and underlying worldviews of a particular discipline or field of inquiry. In our case,
this means knowledge of the current assumptions and values underlying psychology, including
Introduction
3
ideas concerning psychologys proper methods. Sometimes students of psychology are surprised
or even disappointed to discover the assumptions underlying psychologys cherished ideas and
explanations. When brought into the light of day, these fundamental ideas often seem less
compelling and certain than they once did. For this reason, explicating these assumptions and
understanding their implications is the first step in these students becoming critical evaluators of
their discipline.
The second part of this reformulation of critical thinking involves developing ideas and
assumptions that are alternative to our present views. To engage seriously in critical thinking
about psychology, we must seek out and ponder the most credible and convincing alternatives to
psychologys currently favored ideas and methods. Often, the assumptions and guiding values of
mainstream psychology are so familiar that they seem like the only possible premises for our
work. Indeed, they seem more like axioms and truisms than the working assumptions or fruitful
perspectives they are. Knowing there are alternative possibilities, however, allows students to
question the often taken for granted assumptions of their field. This questioning is important
because these assumptions may themselves need to be re-evaluated a possibility that cannot
even be seriously entertained without having alternatives to which to compare present
assumptions.
Illustrating the New Conception of Critical Thinking
To illustrate these issues, consider two examples, one from everyday life and the other
from the world of professional psychology.
Danny and his mother Jill. Consider first the situation of young Danny and his mother
Jill. Jill wants to critically evaluate the problems that Danny is having at school. She spends
considerable time taking a rigorous reasoning approach to the problem. She reads about what
Introduction
4
scientists and psychologists have to say about Dannys behavior and symptoms, and even
investigates recent empirical studies. Jill eventually comes to the conclusion that Danny might
be diagnosed with ADHD (Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder). When she questions her
family physician about this possibility, he immediately confirms Jills suspicions and prescribes
a drug to correct Dannys problem. The importance of rigorous thinking in Jills solution to
her problem is undeniable, both in the process of Jills thinking and in the scientists
investigations of these behaviors.
However, there is a great deal more to a thoroughly critical analysis of this situation.
Truly critical thinking would also examine some of the assumptions made in this process of
diagnosis and prescription as well as consider possible alternative perspectives that might be
helpful to the problem. In fact, recent interview studies show that many people like Danny and
his mother take for granted that the prescribed medication is largely, if not solely, responsible for
the decrease in Dannys ADHD behaviors (e.g., Burchfield & Slife, 2003). In other words, they
make the common but thoroughly debatable assumption that Dannys biology (with the drug
changes) strongly determines these behaviors the assumption of biological determinism. As a
result, Danny believes that he has little or no personal responsibility or choice about his bad
(ADHD) behaviors at school, because personal responsibility and choice are typically associated
with an alternative set of assumptions, namely that Danny has some capacity for free will and
personal responsibility that is usually an important dimension of his behavior. For this reason,
Danny gives up making any effort to control his problematic behaviors.
Dannys mother has similar ideas about her responsibilities as a parent. She assumes, for
instance, that his diagnosis and treatment mean that his problems at school cannot be blamed on
her faulty parenting. After all, she cannot be held responsible for Dannys difficulties if his
Introduction
5
biology is responsible for them. Her experiences with Dannys diagnosis and treatment also
teach her that she has limited parental responsibility for any of Dannys future difficulties,
because neither Danny nor her parenting controls them (see Chapter 6 for more on this
assumption). The point of this illustration is to ask a vital question: Is it not important to
identify and examine such assumptions when diagnosing and treating children for ADHD?
Might not the assumption of biological determinism lead Jill and Danny to overlook other
possibly valuable resources in their struggle for Dannys well-being? Could the tacit assumption
of biological determinism lead them to overlook important dimensions of the problem, and direct
them, inadvertently, to view themselves as somewhat passive and unable to control the situation?
Empirically supported treatments. Consider another example of critical thinking in the heart of
professional psychology. Many psychologists today believe that counselors and psychotherapists should
only use therapeutic strategies that have been critically evaluated (cf. Messer, 2001). According to the
conventional idea of critical thinking in psychology rigorous thinking the proper evaluation of
counseling strategies or techniques consists of employing scientific reasoning and testing to demonstrate
their effectiveness. Indeed, a list of these strategies has now been drawn up as empirically supported
treatments (ESTs), with some psychologists proposing that these should be the only treatments
permitted in psychotherapy (Division 12 Task force, 1995). Should therapeutic approaches be restricted
in this manner?
Many critics of ESTs, including many psychologists, believe that this approach is too narrow,
limited, and mechanical (e.g., Bohart, 1998; Messer, 2001). However, many EST supporters view these
critics as better at protesting the EST movement than stating clearly what is wrong with the movement
or proposing constructively a better alternative. Indeed, from the perspective of many EST advocates,
the critics of ESTs have little to recommend other than an unsystematic, anything goes approach.
Introduction
6
This unsystematic approach appears to return psychology to the same chaotic situation that gave rise to
the use of rigorous thinking and methods in the first place a situation in which psychotherapists were
not properly held accountable. Thus, the critics of ESTs often fan the fires of their own discontent.
This controversy presents a classic case of the need for a truly critical analysis in the expanded
sense recommended in this book. Of course, a complete analysis of this controversy is not appropriate
or even possible here, though aspects of this issue and a number of others like it are examined in some
detail in the chapters that follow (e.g., Chapters 3 and 4). For the purpose of illustration, however, it
may be sufficient here to point to one of the many unexamined, and possibly quite problematic,
assumptions underlying the EST controversy the assumption that effectiveness is needed. Is there any
more frequently used concept in professional psychology than effectiveness? Effectiveness is often
touted as professional psychologys highest ideal, but what is it really?
The Meaning of Effectiveness. Perhaps the core meaning of effectiveness is that some method
or technique reliably and predictably produces some desired end or result. If we want a therapeutic
method to be effective, we must first specify the end or outcome of the technique we desire. The
problem is that merely wanting effectiveness tells us nothing about what ends or goals are truly
worthwhile. There are effective terrorists and thieves just as there are effective teachers and clergy. The
profound differences among these types of effectiveness involve the human and moral quality of the
ends they serve, not their productivity or efficiency. In this sense, rigorous reasoning and empirical
methods are perhaps best suited for assessing the effectiveness of means, not for judging the quality or
worth of ends. Surely a comprehensive approach to understanding and improving human life has to
include both. Surely critical thinking requires an evaluation of the ideas associated with means and
ends, even if the latter are less empirically accessible.
Introduction
7
To take the matter a step further, the enormous emphasis placed on effectiveness in professional
psychology often reflects an assumption that much of everyday life is a matter of maximizing
effectiveness and control over our environments and ourselves. However, many critics in recent decades
including psychologists such as Erich Fromm (1969/41) and John Schumaker (2001) as well as
philosophers such as Christopher Lasch (1991), Jrgen Habermas (1973), Alasdair MacIntyre (1984), to
mention just a few have argued that our emphasis on mastery, control, and cost-benefit (effectiveness)
analyses are actually a major source of emotional problems, mental illness, and relationship problems.
In the minds of these thinkers, mastery and effectiveness are often splendid things, but they need to be
subordinated to more worthy and lasting purposes in living. If psychology does not take such critical
perspectives into account, it runs the risk of inadvertently perpetuating some of the ills it tries to cure.
Of course, everyone has their own values and biases. However, the purpose of this brief
illustration on ESTs is not to argue for one moral outlook or philosophy over another. Rather, it is to
point out that the extensive discussion of effectiveness in psychology is based on a number of
unexamined assumptions and tacit values that are worthy of serious reconsideration. As needed and
helpful as rigorous and scientific thinking is, it is inadequate to the tasks associated with truly critical
thinking. Paul Wachtel (1997), a psychotherapist noted for critical thinking in the fullest sense, put it
this way:
We need a good deal more critical thought about how to conceptualize the issues, about what is
worth knowing, and about the various ways in which what has been observed thus far can be
understood. We need to examine more closely the assumptions that underlie our questions. For
our questions are our destiny; once we have framed a question, the answer already lies in wait,
concealed as the statue is in the sculptors block of marblePsychology has been obsessed with
answers. This book is concerned mainly with questions. (p. xvii)
Introduction
8
This book, too, is concerned mainly with questions. However, as Wachtel observes, good questions
which already frame good answers originate from critical thinking that examines more closely the
assumptions that underlie our questions.
The Literature on Critical Thinking
Does the research literature on critical thinking in psychology address the importance of
unexamined assumptions? How do specialists in this research conceptualize critical thinking?
A review of this literature reveals careful empirical studies as well as instructive theoretical
insights. However, few if any of the investigators working in this area have advocated the need
to think critically about fundamental assumptions. Although these authors invariably endorse the
need to think critically about and thus empirically investigate all sorts of claims (e.g., folk
psychology, advertising), they rarely recommend that the assumptions underlying these
investigations be critically examined.
Many specific texts appear, particularly in their titles, to critically evaluate psycholgys
methods, such as Meltzoffs (1998) Critical Thinking about Research and Benjafields (1994)
Thinking Critically about Research Methods. However, these texts concentrate almost
exclusively on showing how the tools of traditional science can rigorously assess claims and test
hypotheses. They do not cast critical light on the nature of these methods, nor do they bring to
this light their historical context, assumptions, and implications. Critical thinking is thus
couched rather narrowly in terms of rigorous reasoning, namely, the quantity and quality of
empirical support for claims, theories, and therapies. Critical thinking as discussed by
Brookfield (1987) and the authors of this book is largely ignored.
The general literature on critical thinking in psychology also champions rigorous
reasoning (Bensley, 1998; Halpern, 1984; 1998; Halpern & Nummedal, 1995; Lehman, Lempert,
Introduction
9
& Nisbett, 1988; Levy, 1997; McGovern, Furumoto, Halpern, Kimble, & McKeachie, 1991;
Smith, 2002; Stanovich, 1998; Zechmeister & Johnson, 1992). A review of this literature
suggests that scientific analytic reasoning has shaped much of our disciplines consciousness
about the nature of excellent thinking: excellent thinkers are professionals and students who can
reason well about methods, variables, and the logicality of their thinking within the cannons of
empirical science.
This view of excellent thinking is also reflected in the reasoning and methods of many
approaches to psychotherapy and counseling in at least three ways. First, a critical appraisal of
therapeutic methods is almost always considered complete with scientific analytic reasoning,
such as testing their effectiveness through the methods of science (Messer, 2001). Second,
therapeutic methods are themselves thought to be administered rigorously and rationally, as
indicated by the use of therapy manuals and standardized diagnoses (Division 12 Task force,
1995). Third, rigorous reasoning has itself become a standard for identifying client problems and
desired treatment outcomes, such as in cognitive behavioral therapy (e.g., Beck, Rush, Shaw, &
Emery, 1984). A lack of rigorous reasoning is frequently viewed as an important problem for
clients, where they are viewed as irrational, while rigorous reasoning about the conduct of their
lives is often considered a preferred method and outcome of treatment.
If psychologists seek to champion critical thinking and grant it prominent status in the
discipline, why is critical discussion confined to the rigor of reasoning and methods alone?
Analysis of fundamental assumptions is virtually nonexistent in psychologys general textbooks
and the more specific critical thinking literature. On the relatively few occasions that
assumptions are mentioned in this literature, they are treated in only a cursory way (e.g.,
Bensley, 1998; Halpern, 1998). Fundamental questions about the very nature and purpose of
Introduction
10
commonly accepted psychological methods, theories, and therapies simply are not addressed
(e.g., Smith, 2002). These authors seem to assume that rigorous reasoning is all that is required
to deal with the challenges of understanding psychological life and facilitating human
flourishing. Or perhaps critical thinking in the broader sense of this book seems unnecessary to
these authors because the only alternative to careful, rigorous reasoning as they conceive of it
is blind intuition, irrational dogma, or ineffable mysticism.
The authors of this volume believe otherwise. We believe there are profound and
pressing reasons for taking a richer view of critical thinking and making it a routine part of our
work as social scientists and professional psychologists. Restricting ourselves to rigorous
reasoning alone is a fools errand, even in our practical lives. Our difficulties in coping with
some person or problem do not always result from overt tactics or conscious reasoning about the
situation. They may also stem from faulty assumptions about anothers motives or the way the
world works, which a moment of insight or a good word from a friend helps us understand.
Indeed, as we go about our daily lives, we are constantly revising our assumptions (often
realizing that we were making them for the first time), considering alternatives, and beginning to
make progress instead of just spinning our wheels. Critical thinking in this reformulated sense is
part of thoughtful, meaningful living.
Content Overview
To help psychologists advance their discipline in thoughtful and meaningful ways, the
content of this book is organized into six parts, each part corresponding to a major subdiscipline
of psychology: clinical/counseling, social, neuroscience/experimental, cognitive, developmental,
and statistical/methodological. Each part consists of two chapters that critically examine several
important topics or issues within a subdiscipline. Because truly critical thinking involves an
Introduction
11
understanding of both the current and alternative assumptions underlying each topic or issue,
each part consists of two chapters one that excavates current assumptions and one that explores
plausible alternative assumptions.
The book opens with a critical examination of several pivotal issues in the subdiscipline
of clinical/counseling psychology. In the first chapter of this pair, Frank Richardson, himself a
counseling psychologist, identifies tacit assumptions and values that underpin professional
psychology and may be the source of often-discussed problems and blind spots in this field.
Blaine Fowers, also a counseling psychologist, sketches an alternative to psychotherapy
assumptions, drawing on the field of virtue ethics, that portrays personal development,
psychological well-being, and the good life in a fresh and potentially fruitful way.
The chapters of Part 2 address issues in social psychology. To begin this part, social
psychologists Jeff Reber and Lisa Osbeck investigate the strengths and limitations of the
traditional assumptions underlying social psychology topics such as sociality, love, and helping
behavior. In light of these limitations, theoretical psychologist Ed Gantt provides alternatives to
these assumptions by borrowing from social constructionist psychology, hermeneutic
philosophy, and ethical phenomenology.
Part 3 is concerned with several important issues and themes of experimental psychology,
specifically neuroscience. In the first chapter, neuroscientist and pharmacologist Dawson
Hedges collaborates with Colin Burchfield to outline the main current assumptions of
neuroscience research on mental disorders, especially research on depression and
antidepressants. The next chapter contains a proposal for reframing these assumptions of
neuroscience. Theoretical psychologist Brent Slife teams up with neuroscientist Ramona
Introduction
12
Hopkins to discuss how this alternative proposal might better fit neuroscience data and provide
valuable new insights into neuroscience investigations of children diagnosed with ADHD.
The fourth part of the book investigates several key issues in cognitive psychology. In
the first chapter, Robert Bishop, a philosopher of science and mind, explicates the assumptions
underlying current understandings of memory, traditional models of reasoning, and the computer
metaphor for the human mind. In the second chapter, cognitive psychologist Stephen Yanchar
explores interesting and fruitful alternative ways to understand how people experience the world
and engage in activities such as acting and remembering.
Part 5 addresses significant topics in developmental psychology. In the current
assumptions chapter, developmental psychologists Brian Vandenberg and Shawn OConnor use
Piagets theory to cast light on some of the deepest presuppositions of this subdiscipline. They
discuss the broader historical context of cosmological, geological, and biological theories of
development. Developmentalist John Christopher analyzes in the second chapter how our
Western cultural understandings have deeply colored familiar theories of development. He then
outlines an alternative approach that might overcome this Western bias while still speaking to
common features of human development across cultures.
The final part of the book evaluates several pivotal topics in the subdiscipline concerned
with statistics and methods, including hypothesis testing, measurement, and the interpretation of
results. Statistician and methodologist Richard Williams describes the current assumptions that
underlie these notions, while two psychological researchers with expertise in the philosophy of
science, Jeff Sugarman and Jack Martin, propose alternative conceptions of human science
inquiry.
Introduction
13
Suzanne Kirschner, a theoretical psychologist, concludes the book by identifying many of
the common ideas that cut across the chapters. She points to some fascinating thematic
connections both across the chapters describing current assumptions and across the chapters that
discuss alternative conceptions of each subdiscipline. These themes include issues of mind/body
dualism, social atomism, individualism, meaning-centeredness, free will/determinism,
contextualism, and many others. Ultimately, as Dr. Kirschner observes, the purpose of all 12
chapters is to help the students of psychology reflect more deeply upon the premises and
methods of the field.
The authors believe that this reflection will help students in three important ways. First,
students will realize that psychological theories and ideas including those that initially seemed
unquestionable are based on fallible assumptions and subject to critical examination. Second,
students will learn the advantages and disadvantages of available ideas, allowing them to become
sophisticated consumers of these ideas as well as more careful about their own positions on
psychological issues. Third, students will understand these ideas within a broader intellectual
framework, situating psychology within the larger enterprise of science and the humanities.
Introduction
14
References
Beck, A., Rush, A., Shaw, F., & Emery, G. (1984). The cognitive therapy of depression.
New York: Guilford.
Bem, S., & de Jong, H. L. (1997). Theoretical issues in psychology: An introduction.
London: Sage Publications.
Benjafield, J. G. (1994). Thinking critically about research methods. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
Bernstein, R. J. (1983). Beyond objectivism and relativism: Science, hermeneutics, and
praxis. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Bensley, D. A. (1998). Critical thinking in psychology: A unified skills approach.Pacific
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Bohart, A.C., OHara, M., & Leitner, L. M. (1998). Empirically violated treatments:
Disenfranchisement of humanistic and other psychotherapies. Psychotherapy Research, 8, 141-
157.
Bohman, J. (1993). New philosophy of social science. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Brookfield, S. (1987). Developing critical thinkers: Challenging adults to explore
alternative ways of thinking. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
Curd, M., & Cover, J. A. (1998). Philosophy of science: The central issues. New York:
W. W. Norton & Company.
Dick, R. D. (1991). An empirical taxonomy of critical thinking. Journal of Instructional
Psychology, 18, 79-92.
Division 12 Task Force. (1995). Training in and dissemination of empirically validated
psychological treatments: Report and recommendations. The Clinical Psychologist, 48, 3-23.
Introduction
15
Ennis, R. H. (1982). Identifying implicit assumptions. Syntheses, 51, 61-86.
Fromm, E. (1969). Escape from freedom. New York: Avon (Original work published in
1941).
Griggs, R. A., Jackson, S. L., Marek, P., & Christopher, A. N. (1998). Critical thinking
in introductory psychology texts and supplements. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 254-266.
Habermas, J. (1973). Theory and practice. Boston: Beacon Press.
Halpern, D. F. (1984). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains:
Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist,
53, 449-455.
Halpern, D. F., & Nummedal, S. G. (Eds.)(1995). Special issue: Psychologists teach
critical thinking. Teaching of Psychology, 22.
Heiman, G. W. (1995). Research methods in psychology. Boston: Houghton-
Mifflin.
Lasch, C. (1991). The true and only heaven: Progress and its critics. New York: Norton.
Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training
on reasoning: Formal discipline and thinking about everyday-life events. American
Psychologist, 43, 431-442.
Levy, D. A. (1997). Tools of critical thinking: Metathoughts for psychology. Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.
MacIntyre, A. (1984). After Virtue. (2nd Edition). Notre Dame, IN: Notre Dame
University Press.
Introduction
16
McGovern, T. V., Furumoto, L., Halpern, D. F., Kimble, G. A., & McKeachie, W. J.
(1991). Liberal education, study in depth, and the arts and sciences majorpsychology.
American Psychologist, 46, 598-605.
Meltzoff, J. (1998). Critical thinking about research: Psychology and related fields.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2001). Critical thinking: Tools for taking charge of your learning
and your life. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Schumaker, J. (2001). The age of insanity: Modernity and mental health.